© Distribution of this video is restricted by its owner
Transcript ×
Auto highlight
Font-size
00:08 mhm. This is lecture 22 of . We developed this concept of brain

00:15 when we talked about multiple things in course. But we talked about maps

00:21 calcium fluctuations at the synapse and we about synaptic transmission and we talked about

00:29 cortex and somatosensory barrel cortex and rodents how you, when you activate a

00:37 you see a map of activity. there's a structure that represents that whisker

00:43 and there's activity within that barrel that a matter of sensory or touch sensation

00:50 that single whisker. And we saw these maps and some out of sensor

00:56 if they start from a single those maps of activity, they spread

01:03 the adjacent interconnected brain regions. The of activity can be described as a

01:10 wave or a wave of traveling electrochemical through the interconnected neuronal networks. We

01:20 looked at the examples of brain maps the olfactory system and what happens when

01:31 is proceeding certain smiles and we saw certain structure of the olfactory epithelium.

01:38 had a variety of receptor cells olfactory cells that all expressed a slightly different

01:50 protein and that receptor protein was responsive certain voters in the environment. So

02:01 have a variety of these specific receptor receptor cells. In fact we receptor

02:09 that process these olfactory stimuli. We about how a lot of bill factors

02:19 our Turpin's and they can be synthetic and most of our environments. Most

02:27 our experiences we encounter a lot of or nature produced our beans and that

02:35 is a whole map and that map at the level of the olfactory,

02:42 in the glimmering light where you will different sense producing a different map of

02:49 minty versus fruity within the olfactory involved the glamorous life. That information is

02:59 throughout the cortex and into the interconnected regions. And that's why we talked

03:08 how smells. Although people will say a psychological effect of the smell,

03:15 clearly seeing that there's a physiological effect the brain activity which will further influence

03:22 mood, will influence your decision. tell you I'm going to eat this

03:29 we'll tell you maybe I like this or not. So it is not

03:34 psychological, it actually will determine a of new motor output physiology in the

03:41 which is intertwined with the physiology of body. So we have this beautiful

03:50 or smell maps and when we looked the olfactory evolves here. The imaging

04:00 that was used was calcium imaging. that's an important technique especially within the

04:07 evolved because most of the deep polarization the olfactory receptor cells comes from calcium

04:14 chloride channels. And when you measure , you measure activity. So you

04:23 calcium in this case you measure not increases in calcium but you're also measuring

04:30 deep polarization of the membrane potential. these maps that are created and the

04:43 in which they travel are similar, they're unique in all of us.

04:51 smells and how we perceive the smells dependent upon what we've learned, not

04:57 the structure that we have built but also the environments that we have

05:01 exposed two. And I was also you about the smell maps of the

05:11 , which is an artistic rendition. I challenge the class yesterday saying what

05:22 blind people had an additional map of environments of their community, of their

05:32 ? And those were the smell smell that they could read in braille.

05:40 what maps do blind people read So there is no sound map

05:52 there is no tape that I pick or recording or app that tells me

05:57 is the sound map for you of , it doesn't exist. So there

06:03 braille maps. But braille maps are complicated. There's a lot of things

06:13 there and this is a very simplified map that could be laid on the

06:18 , braille map also in braille. when we talk about these things and

06:22 think, oh it's just fun that could be really serious signs behind

06:27 could be helping another tool for disabled . For example, if they had

06:32 smell map of the building, but could also rely in addition to the

06:37 map because maybe they're special orientation is . So they are not sure which

06:45 they really turned and with those people smelling having another sense and relying on

06:54 is another tool that they could basically by. So yeah. So what

07:00 the change? That's a great Yes. So the direction of minimal

07:09 and if you're inside you know sometimes heater will kick in and sometimes

07:14 C. And this will change Maybe the smell. So the way

07:19 move through the vans. Yeah, there will be variability for sure.

07:24 then at the same time, you that you have the locations of sewage

07:30 and those typically don't change. And this is this is there permanently so

07:36 infrastructural things that have strong smells, and maybe you could say in the

07:44 this will smell like flowers and then fall it this will smell like

07:49 you know something like that. So think there's there's something to it.

07:55 just throwing all of these ideas out for you guys because you're the future

07:58 this world and you're gonna be doing of these things in the future.

08:03 go away, please thank you. . Alright, so now we're going

08:11 look at another very interesting technique that allows you to experimentally measure activity,

08:20 activity, neuronal number in deep hyper polarization. And this technique is

08:29 voltage sensitive dye images. And before explained to you this technique, I

08:34 to tell you that in general when talk about imaging, we're talking about

08:41 levels at which this activity can be . So when we're talking about macroscopic

08:50 , we're talking about imaging primary somatosensory S. S one activity And Primary

09:01 Cortex V one Activity. This is . Or if you may holistic image

09:09 the large area swaths of the brain are activated, then if you tune

09:16 one region like the one you are at this Mezza Skop IQ level.

09:25 if you know and recall the anatomy B1, there's a certain structure in

09:31 cortex recall that cortex is a six structure so laminar and also it has

09:40 . So it's columnar laminar and columnar . And within the cortex you also

09:46 a certain circuit and arrangement. 80-90% the cells in the new cortex are

09:55 to program in all cells 10-20% are into neurons. But you've also learned

10:03 in this course that the diversity in cellular subtype population comes from the inhibitory

10:12 . So excited to results are mostly to re long range production cells that

10:19 different regions of the brain and inhibitory are locally controlling these excitatory cells and

10:28 is a variety of these inhibitory cells how they can control the excitatory cells

10:33 how that output is going to be . This approach is circuits centric,

10:40 for circuit centric approach, you want know the players that are involved self

10:46 that are involved connectivity and the rules which that circuit functions a rule for

10:56 of feedback inhibition when there is a of excitation. Those unsanitary cells will

11:03 inhibitory cells and the inhibitory cells will the same excitatory cells. So there

11:10 arrangement cellular arrangement, connectivity in that . And there are rules by which

11:18 circuits function. There's excitation limit, inhibition Gaba, there's color,

11:27 pan, african, serotonin and there rules by which these circuits function.

11:33 you can get to this circuit centric and understanding understanding what understanding what these

11:42 do during a particular phenomenon. Let's your visual cortex is activated by an

11:51 and you want to know what are inhibitory subtypes of cells doing when they're

11:58 when they're silent. What are you ourselves doing at what time and when

12:03 that information being communicated and propagated into adjacent interconnected brain regions. If you

12:13 that level of understanding on a circuit , the connectivity, the communication between

12:19 cells. You can now go down the cellular level. So there are

12:27 techniques that will allow you to go to a single cell level, that

12:35 huge because you can image activity in neuron and unlike recording electrically from two

12:44 three maximum, I think the record recording simultaneously. Also recordings in vitro

12:51 the slides from eight cells simultaneously is record. I believe in the

12:56 Although it's not in the Guinness world . If we are doing imaging when

13:03 imaging circuits and you get down to you the level with the imaging techniques

13:08 have capability of tracking activity and hundreds cells at the same and thousands of

13:14 at the same time. That's something the recording electrode cannot do. You

13:19 cannot stick 200 electorates underneath a small and you certainly cannot do it in

13:28 . You cannot stick so many electrodes pick up intracellular activity. I'm not

13:34 that there is no electrode race of to the brain. If you follow

13:40 of the multi electrode eraser called, would be implanted, they will have

13:45 of electrodes for experimental purposes. But we're talking imaging imaging activity and hundreds

13:52 thousands of units if you even have resolution. And if you have certain

14:03 , certain tools you have certain dyes certain tools you can get to the

14:09 cellular level, you can get to level of a single synapse of a

14:18 button. Of the synapse of the drive of the dendritic spine and you

14:27 get to that sub cellular level. you need to use really powerful

14:34 If on the macro side, you really need to magnify you know,

14:40 talking about four x four times magnifying signal that you're looking at. So

14:46 magnification on this sub cellular side now have to have really powerful microscopes that

14:56 have very high spatial resolution. And when you talk about functional neural activity

15:08 , you will often hear this term space or temporal resolution or spatial temporal

15:17 of activity special. And temporal when talking about resolution, spatial temporal resolution

15:26 looking at resolution in space. How are these microscopes? How powerful are

15:34 cameras connected to the microscopes? It's only the microscope that image comes through

15:42 camera, a digital camera that is to a computer so that you can

15:47 these beautiful images on the computer. how powerful is the camera? How

15:55 pixels does the camera have? In words, how powerful is the objective

16:01 on the microscope? You're magnifying It's an objective. Your magnifying four

16:07 your magnifying 100 times your magnifying 1000 now you're getting down into the sub

16:13 level. But how good is your ? How many pixels the camera mounted

16:19 your microscope? How many pixels does have? Does it have 10

16:24 What does it mean if the camera 10 pixels here is an image of

16:30 apple and it has like some interesting here and some color blah blah blah

16:37 . Maybe this is like something some Right? And now you have your

16:46 because 1, 2, 3, , 5, 6, 7,

16:51 , 10, 11, 12 It means whatever is in this pixel

16:56 gonna get averaged over and you're not see much of the detail that was

17:02 here. Have a camera that has lot more pixels. You will get

17:11 higher spatial resolution. So how many does your iphone have anybody bothered to

17:20 ? Probably not the last five Nobody bothered. But you used to

17:24 these really expensive cameras like Canon Pay To get like 12 megapixels and things

17:37 that. And it has been replaced cell phone cameras that approximately the same

17:44 . Another way of thinking it is less spatial resolution that has the blurrier

17:49 the image gets, The more spatial has the more detail you can

17:55 So that's for spatial resolution. Now camera that is processing the spatial information

18:04 has to have speed sampling speed. is what is your video camera sample

18:11 the phone? Phone has like 2000 . We only use three. We

18:18 even know what it does. So probably 30 frames per second. You'll

18:24 30 FPs that stands 30 frames per . What does that mean? That

18:30 that whatever happens in one second of is segmented into 30 windows. So

18:39 I'm reaching over one second alright it be segmented into 30 samples of this

18:49 . What's that? And very slow have a 60 frames per second is

18:56 to be twice as fast you have T. V. Maybe you guys

19:03 some things there's this program the It's somewhat scientific but they take different

19:09 and they have super fast high resolution so they can show you the plumes

19:15 smoke or explosions of breaking glass. you see that with 30 frames per

19:22 ? No you need super super fast . So the things that have to

19:29 if you're imaging fast activity, if imaging fast activity on the single cell

19:35 , you better have a really powerful , you better have really good spatial

19:40 . If you want to image electrical action potentials E. P.

19:44 P. S. What's the duration an action potential millisecond to milliseconds.

19:52 many is that a second? One is 1001. of a second.

20:03 means if you wanted to capture that millisecond. Long action potential you have

20:09 have at least twice at least. have to take two samples of that

20:15 potential And that would be 2000 So two kilohertz. Now if you

20:23 to see all of the details as points along this action potential. If

20:28 just took two measurements it might be in here depending on your speed.

20:34 is your sampling rate. If you the sampling rate. Now you can

20:39 the action potential. It's every what these sports? Now? Now you

20:48 have this really beautiful representation. So have to match that whatever you're gonna

20:54 in the system whenever dies you're gonna . But these guys can track as

21:01 with activity electrical activity in the movement the dye that the cameras can pick

21:07 up and the cameras can process it . The other thing that happens is

21:12 after you collected the data and a of these experiments are very difficult and

21:18 eunice and you have to have Sometimes these dyes are genetically expressed in

21:25 . You have to have tissue prepared . You know, it's usually 23

21:30 , eight hours of work to get , you know, another eight hours

21:34 get experiment done. We're going home . You have four hours of

21:42 It's gonna probably take you five times long to analyze that data at least

21:50 extract the most meaningful information. So you acquired after you matched everything up

21:55 is post experimental processing of data. may want to filter the noise out

22:02 electrical signals. You may want to the noise out in optical signals.

22:06 may want to zoom in on an of interest and zoom in on another

22:10 of interest. If you zoom maybe you want to go down to

22:14 cell resolution. If you have that stored and the speeds that are necessary

22:21 you to understand that data. So these are different levels of

22:27 And this is one advantage of genetically voltage indicators, imaging is that it

22:36 cover multiple spatial scales, resolution coverage from the whole brain to dendritic

22:42 So there are certain dies. And this case it's genetically expressed voltage

22:49 Yeah. So let's let's understand. are these voltage dies and how they

22:55 . And these voltage dies are really for imaging cortical dynamics. And you

23:02 image these cortical dynamics in vivo, is in the whole animal. And

23:07 vitro when you're imaging activity using these sensitive dyes, you're still typically imaging

23:16 from approximately 100 to 200 micro meters the surface after activity. Now you

23:26 say okay. But what if I a really powerful microscope and that microscope

23:32 go down and image deeper? if you had come focal microscopy you

23:37 go down a little bit deeper and can focus in on a certain focal

23:43 . It's called in the microscope a bit deeper. But still most of

23:47 things that you're going to observe is to be on the surface. And

23:52 remember when we saw these beautiful orientation columns and their colors blue red,

24:00 . And I said that these are of the different cells reacting to different

24:04 in the optical columns in the visual . Those experiments were done with voltage

24:10 with imaging voltage sensitive dyes. So way this works is these dyes,

24:16 can be genetically expressed within the membranes in this example in this experiment.

24:22 another one that I will show you dyes are embedded into the plasma

24:27 So you can apply the dye on surface of the cortex so you can

24:31 the dye on the surface of the . And those guys are like these

24:35 squiggly warms and they will penetrate into plasma membranes of the cells and these

24:41 warms. They have a certain confirmation them and they have certain reflective or

24:49 properties. That means that if you a light on it like a fluorescent

24:52 it's gonna reflect at a certain wavelength indicates it's confirmation all state. Now

25:01 voltage crosses through the ion channels and is deep polarization across plasma membrane,

25:09 dyes and the plasma membrane will change confirmation. The warm is gonna bend

25:15 a different direction and as they change confirmation, the reflective properties are going

25:22 change. So with deep polarization they change their confirmation in one direction and

25:28 reflected is going to increase. So areas where there's deep polarization and the

25:34 change are going to be indicated optically active areas. And then when the

25:41 hyper polarizes, this quickly dies will into a different confirmation. And as

25:47 shine the light they will reflect it different ways indicating that there is co

25:54 or silence or inhibition in those particular . So the wonderful thing here that

26:02 shown is that there is a blue and the red trace in this diagram

26:11 and the blue and the red traits virtually indistinguishable. However, one of

26:18 traces is an electrical potential recording with electrode. Just like we studied traditional

26:26 physiological recordings of E. P. . P. S. And action

26:29 throughout this course. The second color is an optical recording that is picked

26:36 by this very fast camera. So that tells you is and I'm not

26:43 ask him to test which one is , because I can't remember which one

26:46 which. The fact is that both sensitive dye imaging is directly correlated to

26:56 changes of the electrical membrane potential. it's very fast. So it can

27:05 up a PS PS. It can pick up action potentials, individual action

27:12 . And so this is a an where you have a macroscopic. So

27:18 not really magnifying it that much. this case, you're not as much

27:22 about a single cell resolution, but want to see those pinwheel cortical Structures

27:28 neurons that are responsive to one Orientation the other. So now you have

27:34 window, you insert an electrode, is your electrical recording into the tissue

27:41 neuron. And you have the microscope is only a portion of the microscope

27:48 has shown here that is imaging inside window here and the data that is

27:55 collected by this fast camera. So cameras to capture action potential activity

28:03 Think about the speed and the scales be in kilohertz. You want to

28:08 four kilohertz, 10 kilohertz of optical . You want to have thousands of

28:16 of resolution in that the data files of pixels, 10 kHz of imaging

28:26 for two minutes. It's like a GB of data. You're running experiment

28:32 four hours. You can you can can count how much data storage you

28:36 in the computers and files and transfers backups and things like that. So

28:43 a lot of data. But so this is an experiment and you have

28:47 visual stimulator for the animal and instead just recording from an individual cell like

28:54 would in the orientation columns, spoke one cell. What is this cell

28:58 to this orientation or that orientation? . The following day and I'm going

29:03 focus on other sell it takes me hours to get there this orientation without

29:08 . So this was the evolution of the primary visual cortex and understanding orientation

29:14 is literally poking cell by cell one one. Until we had imaging techniques

29:21 the intrinsic optical signal revealed the ocular columns. Until you had more spatial

29:27 in the microscopes and the cameras and dyes that were able to reveal the

29:33 column structure and the cortex that we in the visual cortex. And so

29:39 is the precise experiment. And these the multi sensitive dyes that we're talking

29:46 . Let me make sure I So now we're going to watch movie

29:54 one as I call. Okay and number one is called epileptic brain

29:57 And these are the recordings that were in the movie that was produced by

30:02 and my student. And I will as we watch this movie. But

30:06 should be able to recognize some of structures like the hippocampus stimulation of hippocampus

30:13 you can read along too 40 hertz stimulation up in the competition. This

30:33 thing is a stimulating electric. It produce currents of stimulus through the spread

30:43 the activity. It travels in a despise what we call the temporal

30:57 Green is like clumps of cells. don't have a singing resolution here.

31:01 these little mountains represent to too few each per pixel. Now it's the

31:07 stimulation but it's a pill epic It's the same stimulus that gets

31:13 But the tissue, the environment of tissue has been made to be chemically

31:20 pro epileptic broken balls. Same Now you can see that. First

31:28 all you're seeing a lot of red is a lot of signal. A

31:36 of activity here. That signal is . It's huge. It's no more

31:41 precise. It's spacious temporarily has been , reorganized into a big med puddle

31:49 activity. This is electrical activity and persistent that way to standing there students

31:59 the same duration stimulus as a first . But now you can see this

32:04 epileptic wave of activity. Standing this without stimulation without stimulation you can observe

32:13 activity. You will see that there's burst of activity that generates right there

32:22 it propagates over there. This is hippocampus, this is dente gyros,

32:28 is C. A. Three and is C A C A one area

32:31 we looked at earlier in the course we studied the diversity of the

32:35 This is individual cortex. You guys the structure of the cortex? Let

32:41 remind you. This is the superficial . So this would be layer

32:46 This is the most deep layers. would be layer six. What happens

32:51 is activity comes from sub cortical layers it moves up the column because you

32:57 a column of structures. If you you have inter cortical loops and it

33:01 be activity coming into four going into . What happens in two? Three

33:08 spreads laterally. Okay, it spreads . It can then travel back into

33:12 column. Can get communicated back into cortex. This is an epileptic form

33:18 but it highlights the cortical connectivity that talked throughout the course. And so

33:24 you have this burst originating deep, traveling up the column, it's spreading

33:31 through layers 23. This activity it's back into the column and as it

33:38 back into the column it breaks up little pieces. So if there is

33:45 map or smell or different odors, also a map for regular activity.

33:52 a map for thoughts. There's a for emotions and there's a map for

33:58 and seizures. And this is a of epilepsy and seizures. There can

34:06 a lot of mathematical studying that can done on these maps. For

34:12 you can overlay the contours of activity the structure. This is just the

34:21 of that previous activity and we're traveled most dominant activity. You can make

34:27 drawing underneath it of the cortical structures the layers and the columns. You

34:32 mathematically reduce, deduce these things. are very fast waves of activity excited

34:41 an inhibitor activity that travel. yeah. Red, blue, red

34:57 excitation inhibition, excitation, inhibition, inhibition. These waves are passing and

35:06 this is a little bit of artistic , chaotic orchestra of ripples, ripples

35:14 these very fast waves of activity That about 200 Hz per second. But

35:21 have different rhythms of activity. We some slower waves of activity and faster

35:26 of activity depending on whatever you're processing the thought pattern maybe or the output

35:32 . Well just imagine that these are thoughts and these are your brain maps

35:38 this artistic rendition as you listen to music. Mm hmm mm hmm mm

35:46 hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm hmm

35:50 hmm hmm hmm hmm. This would our auditory neurons being activated, auditory

36:06 being activated. These waves being translated around between different areas of the

36:20 So that's the end of this. I have to give credit to uh

36:26 to my graduate student who performed these in New Palm Hazara. He did

36:33 PhD in my lab here using electrophysiology these fast voltage imaging sensitive voltage sensitive

36:41 . They're called voltage sensitive dyes because sensitive to voltage. So you're imaging

36:46 voltage because these dyes are sensitive to . So he perfected and performed these

36:53 here that you have a judge then his postdoc at thomas Jefferson University and

36:59 back to India where he is originally as a faculty. So, so

37:06 is the mom knows I'm still playing video to to the students. Thank

37:16 very much. Thank you very Um this video won the third place

37:23 at U. Of H for three renditions of scientific data. So we

37:29 we came up with this video because wanted to participate in the competition and

37:34 won the third place with it which really cool and it was completely different

37:39 everybody's everybody else's three dimensional graphs and that were presented and I think maybe

37:46 music helped us get the third Okay. So we're we're venturing into

37:54 really detailed understanding of the brain connectivity the brain maps. We already talked

38:01 plasticity. We already talked about critical period of development. We talked

38:08 some amount of sensory information. We about learned things that you learned during

38:14 plasticity and we're gonna watch today to . It's about 20 minutes longer,

38:20 minutes, 23 minutes long. So may not talk much during the

38:24 I may review what we watch but it's the three symptoms syndromes and

38:31 will write them down to stop as discusses them. Perhaps one of the

38:36 ted talks in neuroscience today by dr . Um what is um chris pointed

38:48 , I study the human brain the and structure of the human brain and

38:52 just want you to think for a about what this entails Here is this

38:57 of Jelly, Mass of Jelly, can hold in the palm of your

39:02 and it can contemplate the vastness of space. It can contemplate the meaning

39:08 infinity, and it can contemplate itself on the meaning of infinity. And

39:13 is this peculiar recursive quality that we self awareness which I think is the

39:19 Grail of neuroscience of neurology and hopefully we'll understand how that happens.

39:26 so how do you study this mysterious ? I mean, you have 100

39:31 nerve cells, little wisps of protoplasm with each other and from this activity

39:37 the whole spectrum of abilities that we human nature and human consciousness. How

39:42 this happen? Well, there are ways of approaching the functions of the

39:46 brain. One approach. The one use mainly is to look at patients

39:51 have sustained damage to a small region the brain. There's been a genetic

39:55 in a small region of the What then happens is not and across

40:00 board, reduction in all your mental , a sort of blunting of your

40:04 ability. What you get is a selective loss of one function with other

40:08 being preserved intact. And this gives some confidence in asserting that that part

40:13 the brain is somehow involved in mediating function. So you can then map

40:17 onto structure and then find out what circuitry is doing to generate that particular

40:23 . So that's what we're trying to . So let me give you a

40:26 striking examples of this. I'm giving three examples six minutes each during this

40:32 . The first example is an extraordinary called Cat Grass syndrome. If you

40:37 at the first slide, then that's temporal lobes, frontal lobes, parietal

40:42 gave the lobes that constitute the And if you look tucked away inside

40:48 inner surface of the temporal lobes, can't see that is a little structure

40:52 effusive form gyros and that's been called face area in the brain because when

40:56 damaged, you can no longer recognize faces, you can still recognize them

41:01 their voice. Oh yeah, that's but you can't look at their face

41:05 know who it is that you can't recognize yourself in the mirror. I

41:09 , you know it is, it's because when you wink it may winks

41:11 you know it's a mirror, but don't really recognize yourself as as

41:17 Okay, now that syndrome is well is caused by damage to the face

41:20 Honduras. But there's another rare so rare in fact, that very

41:24 physicians have heard about it, Not neurologists, this is called the cab

41:28 delusion and that is a patient who's completely normal to have a head injury

41:34 out of coma otherwise completely normal. looks at his mother and says this

41:40 exactly like my mother, this but she's an impostor, she's some

41:43 woman pretending to be my mother. why does this happen? Why would

41:48 that this person is perfectly lucid and in all other respects. But when

41:51 sees his mother, his delusion kicks says not mother. Now, the

41:55 common interpretation of this, which you in older psychiatry textbooks is a Freudian

42:01 and that is that this chap and same argument applies to women by the

42:05 , but I'll just talk about guys you were a little baby and a

42:09 baby. You had a strong sexual to your mother. This is the

42:12 called EuroPass complex of Freud. I'm saying I believe this, but this

42:16 the standard Freudian view. And then you grow up, the cortex develops

42:22 inhibits these latent sexual urges towards your . Thank God. Otherwise we'd all

42:27 sexually aroused when you saw your And then what happens is there's a

42:32 to your head, damaging the allowing these latent sexual urges to emerge

42:38 to the surface and suddenly and inexplicably find yourself being sexually aroused by your

42:43 . He said, my God, this is my mom, how come

42:45 being sexually turned on? She's some woman, She's an impostor. The

42:49 interpretation that makes sense to your damaged . This never made much sense to

42:55 this argument. It's very ingenious as Freudian arguments are okay, didn't make

43:02 sense, much sense because I have the same delusion, a patient having

43:06 same delusion about his pet poodle, say doctor, this is not

43:12 it looks exactly like Fifi, but some other dog right now you try

43:17 the Freudian explanation that you have to talking about the latent bestiality and all

43:23 or some such thing, which is absurd. Of course. Now,

43:27 really going on? So to explain curious disorder, we look at the

43:31 and functions of the normal visual pathways the brain. Normally visual signals come

43:36 into the eyeballs go to the visual in the brain. There are in

43:39 30 areas in the back of your concerned with just vision. And after

43:43 all that, the message goes to small structure called effusive form Gyros um

43:49 you perceive faces. There are neurons that are sensitive to faces. You

43:53 call it the face area of the , right? I talked about that

43:57 . Now, when that area's you lose the ability to see

44:00 right? But from that area, message cascades into a structure called the

44:05 in the limbic system, the emotional of the brain and that structure called

44:10 amygdala gauges the emotional significance of what looking at. Is it prey?

44:14 it predator? Is it mate? is it something absolutely trivial? Like

44:19 piece of lint or a piece of or or I don't want to point

44:22 that but or a shoe or something that. Okay. Which you can

44:26 ignore. So if the amygdala is and there's something important, the messages

44:31 cascade into the autonomic nervous system. heart starts beating faster, You start

44:36 to dissipate the heat that you're going create from exerting muscular exertion. And

44:41 fortunate because you can put two electrodes your palm and measure the change in

44:45 resistance produced by sweating. So I determine when you're looking at something whether

44:50 excited or whether you're aroused or Ok. And I'll get to that

44:54 a minute. So my idea was this chap looks at an object,

45:00 he looks at his any object for matter, it goes to the visual

45:04 and however and it's processed in the injuries and you recognize it as a

45:08 plant or a table or your mother that matter. Okay. And then

45:13 message cascades into the amygdala and then down the autonomic nervous system. But

45:18 in this chap, that wire that from the amygdala to the limbic

45:22 the emotional core of the brain is by the accident. So because the

45:27 farmers intact the chap can still recognize mother and says, oh yeah,

45:31 looks like my mother. But because wire is cut to the emotional

45:35 But how come it was my I don't experience a warmth or terror

45:40 the case may be. Right, . And therefore he says, how

45:46 I account for this inexplicable lack of . This can't be my mother with

45:50 strange woman pretending to be my How do you test this?

45:53 what you do is if you take one of you here and put you

45:56 front of a screen and measure your skin response and show pictures on the

46:01 , I can measure how you sweat you see an object like a table

46:05 an umbrella, of course you don't . If I show you a picture

46:08 a lion or a tiger or a , you start sweating right and believe

46:13 or not? If I show you picture of your mother, I'm talking

46:16 normal people. You start sweating. don't even have to be jewish.

46:20 hmm. Now what happens? What if you show this patient, you

46:26 the patient and show him pictures on screen and measure his galvanic skin response

46:31 and chairs and lint. Nothing As in normal people. But when

46:36 show him a picture of his the galvanic skin response is flat.

46:40 no emotional reaction to his mother because wire going from the visual areas to

46:45 emotional centers is cut so his vision normal because the visual areas are

46:50 his emotions are normal. He'll he'll cry and so on and so

46:53 . But the wire from vision to is cut and therefore he has this

46:58 that his mother is an impostor. a lovely example of what the sort

47:01 thing. We do take a seemingly incomprehensible neuro psychiatrist syndrome and say

47:07 the standard Freudian view is wrong. in fact, you can come up

47:10 a precise explanation in terms of no anatomy of the brain. By the

47:14 , if this patient then goes and phones from an adjacent room phones him

47:21 he picks up the phone and wow, mom, how are

47:24 Where are you? There's no delusion the phone, then she approaches him

47:28 an hour. He says, who you? You look just like my

47:31 . Okay. The reason is there's separate pathway going from the hearing centers

47:35 the brain to the emotional centers and not being cut by the accident.

47:40 this explains why with a phone he his mother. No problem. When

47:44 sees it in person, he he says, it's an impostor.

47:49 , how is all this complex circuit he set up in the brain?

47:52 it nature genes or is it And we approach this problem by considering

47:56 curious syndrome called phantom limb. And all know what a phantom limb is

48:02 an arm is amputated or a leg amputated for gangrene or you lose it

48:06 war. For example, in the war, it's now a serious

48:10 You continue to vividly feel the presence that missing arm and that's called a

48:14 arm or a phantom leg. In , you can get a phantom with

48:17 any part of the body, believe or not, Even with internal

48:21 I've had patients with the uterus removed who have a phantom uterus, including

48:29 menstrual cramps at the appropriate time of month. And in fact, one

48:34 asked me the other day do they phantom PMS subject ripe for scientific

48:40 but we haven't pursued that. now, the next question is,

48:44 can you learn about phantom limbs by experiments. One of the things we

48:48 was about half the patients with phantom claim that they can move the

48:52 It'll pat his brother on the It'll answer the phone when it

48:56 it'll wave goodbye. These are very , vivid sensations. Patient's not

49:01 He knows that the arm is not , but nevertheless, it's a compelling

49:05 experience for the patient. But about half the patients, this doesn't

49:10 . The phantom limb, they'll But doctor, the phantom limb is

49:14 , it's fixed in a clenched spasm excruciatingly painful. If only I could

49:18 it, maybe the pain will be . Now why would a phantom limb

49:22 paralyzed? It sounds like an When we look at the case

49:27 what we found was these people with paralyzed phantom limbs. The original arm

49:32 paralyzed because of the peripheral nerve The actual nerve supplying the arm was

49:38 was cut by, say, a accident. So the patient had an

49:41 arm which is painful in a sling a few months or a year.

49:46 then in a misguided attempt to get of the pain and the arm,

49:49 sergeant amputate the arm and then you a phantom arm with the same

49:54 Right? And this is a serious problem patients become depressed? Some of

49:59 are driven to suicide. Okay, how do you treat this syndrome.

50:04 , why do you get a paralyzed limb? And I looked at the

50:06 sheet. I found that they had actual arm. The nerves supplying the

50:11 had been cut and the actual arm been paralyzed and lying in a sling

50:16 several months before the amputation. And pain then gets carried over into the

50:24 itself. Why does this happen when arm was intact but paralyzed? The

50:29 sends commands to the arm, the of the brain saying move, but

50:32 getting visual feedback, saying no no move, no move,

50:38 And this gets wired into the circuitry the brain and we call this learned

50:44 . The brain learns because of this in associative link, that the comm

50:49 command to move the arm creates a of a paralyzed arm. And then

50:54 you populate the arm, this learned carries over into the into your body

51:00 and into your phantom. Okay, how do you help these patients?

51:05 do you unlearn the learned paralysis so can relieve him of this excruciating clenching

51:11 of the phantom arm. Well we what if you now send the command

51:16 the phantom but give him visual feedback it's obeying his commands. Maybe you

51:21 relieve the phantom pain. The phantom . How do you do that?

51:25 , virtual reality, but that costs of dollars. So I hit on

51:28 way of doing this for $3. don't tell my funding agencies.

51:35 what you do is you create what call a mirror box. You have

51:38 cardboard box with a mirror in the and then you put the phantom.

51:42 my first patient derek came in. had his arm amputated 10 years

51:46 He had a break in revulsion. the nerves were cut and the arm

51:50 paralyzed lying in a sling for a . And then the arm was

51:53 He had a phantom arm excruciatingly painful he couldn't move. It was a

51:57 phantom long. So he came there I gave him a mirror like that

52:01 a box, okay? Which I the mirror box, right? And

52:05 patient puts his phantom left arm which clenched and inspire them on the left

52:10 of the mirror and the normal hand the right side of the mirror and

52:13 the same posture, The clenched posture looks inside the mirror. And what

52:19 the experience? He looks at the being resurrected because he's looking at the

52:24 of the normal arm in the mirror it looks like this phantom has been

52:28 . Now, I said, now wiggle your phantom your real fingers or

52:33 your real fingers while looking in the . He's going to get the visual

52:37 that the phantom is moving right, obvious. But the astonishing thing is

52:41 patient then says, oh my my phantom is moving again and the

52:45 . The clenching phantom is relieved. remember my first patient who came

52:50 Okay, my first patient came in he looked in the mirror and I

52:57 , look at your reflection of your . He's and he started giggling so

53:01 can see my phantom. But he's stupid. He knows it's not

53:04 He knows it's a mirror reflection, it's a vivid sensory experience.

53:08 I said, move your normal hand phantom, He said, oh,

53:11 can't move my phantom. You know it's painful. I said move your

53:14 hand. And he says, oh God, my phantom is moving

53:17 I don't believe this. And my is being relieved. Ok? And

53:21 I said close your eyes and close eyes and move your normal hand.

53:24 , nothing ! It's clenched again. , open your eyes ! Oh my

53:27 ! Oh my God, it's moving . So, he's like a kid

53:29 a candy store. So I OK, this proves my theory about

53:35 paralysis and the critical role of visual . But I'm not going to get

53:38 nobel prize for getting somebody to move phantom limb. Mm hmm. Completely

53:45 ability. If you think about But then I started realizing maybe other

53:50 of paralysis that you see. In in in neurology like stroke focal

53:56 So there may be a learned component this, which you can overcome with

53:59 simple device of using a mirror. I said, look, derek.

54:03 , first of all, the guy just go around carrying a mirror to

54:06 his pain. I said, Derek, take it home and practice

54:09 it for a week or two, after repeated practice, you can dispense

54:13 the mirror, unlearn the paralysis and moving your paralyzed arm and then relieve

54:18 of pain. So he said, . And he took it home.

54:20 said, Look, it's after $2, take it home. So

54:23 took it home and after two weeks phones me and he said,

54:26 you're not gonna believe this. I what? He said, it's

54:30 I said, what's gone? I maybe the mirror box was gone.

54:34 said, No, No, You know this phantom I've had for

54:36 last 10 years it's disappeared. And said, I got worried. I

54:41 , my God, I mean, changed this guy's body image. What

54:44 human subjects, ethics, and all that? And I said,

54:46 does this bother you? He said , last three days I've not had

54:50 phantom arm and therefore no phantom elbow , no clenching, no phantom forearm

54:56 . All those pains are gone But the problem is I still have

54:59 phantom fingers dangling from the shoulder and box doesn't reach. So can you

55:05 the design and put it on my so I can, you know,

55:08 this and eliminate my phantom fingers. thought it was some kind of

55:13 This happened is because the brain is with tremendous sensory conflict. It's getting

55:18 from vision saying the phantom is On the other hand, there's no

55:21 reception, muscle signals saying that there no arm, right? And your

55:26 command saying there is an arm. because of this conflict, the brain

55:30 to hell with it, There is phantom, there is no arm,

55:32 ? It goes into a sort of . It gates the signals. And

55:35 the arm disappears, the bonus is pain disappears because you can't have disembodied

55:41 floating out there in space. that's the bonus. Now, this

55:45 has been tried on dozens of patients other groups in Helsinki. So it

55:48 prove to be valuable as a treatment phantom pain and indeed people have tried

55:53 for stroke rehabilitation stroke you normally think as damage to the fibers, nothing

55:58 can do about it. But it out some component of stroke paralysis is

56:02 learned paralysis and maybe that component can overcome using mirrors. This has also

56:08 through clinical trials, helping lots and of patients. Okay, let me

56:13 gears now to the third part of talk, which is about another curious

56:17 called synesthesia just discovered by Francis Galton the 19th century. He was a

56:22 of Charles Darwin. He pointed out certain people in the population who are

56:27 completely normal has the following peculiarity every they see a number. It's colored

56:33 is blue, Seven is yellow. is chartreuse. Nine is indigo.

56:39 , bear in mind these people are normal in other respects, or C

56:44 . Sometimes tones evoke color. C is blue. F Sharp is

56:48 Another tone might be yellow. Why does this happen? It's called

56:53 Galton called it synesthesia a mingling of senses in us. All the senses

56:58 distinct. These people muddle up their . Why does this happen? Another

57:02 aspects of this problem are very Synesthesia runs in families. So,

57:06 said, this is a hereditary A genetic basis, secondly, as

57:10 as these years about, and this what gets me to my point about

57:14 main theme of this election is about . Synesthesia is eight times more common

57:19 artists, poets, novelists, and creative people than in the general

57:23 Why would that be? I'm going answer that question has never been answered

57:27 . Okay, what is synesthesia? causes it will want. There are

57:31 theories. One theory is they're just . That's not really a scientific

57:35 So, you can forget about Okay, Another theory Is there acid

57:38 and potheads? Right. There may some truth to this because it's much

57:42 common here in the Bay Area than san Diego. Okay, now,

57:47 third theory is that, Well, ask ourselves what's really going on in

57:52 . Alright. So, but the area and the number area right next

57:56 each other in the brain in the form gyros. So we said there's

58:00 accidental cross wiring between color and numbers the brain. So every time you

58:05 a number you see a corresponding And that's why you get synesthesia now

58:11 it. Why does this happen? would there be crossed wires? And

58:13 people remember I said it runs in . That gives you the clue.

58:17 that is there is an abnormal gene the gene that causes this abnormal cross

58:23 in all of us. It turns we are born with everything wired to

58:27 else. So every brain region is to every other region. And these

58:31 trimmed down to create the characteristic modular of the adult brain. So there's

58:37 gene causing this trimming. And if gene mutates then you get deficient trimming

58:42 adjacent brain areas. And if it's number and color, you get number

58:46 synesthesia. If it's been toned in , you get tone color synesthesia so

58:49 so good. Now, what if gene is expressed everywhere in the

58:53 So everything is cross connected? think about what artists, novelists and

58:59 have in common the ability to engage metaphorical thinking, linking seemingly unrelated ideas

59:06 as it is the east and Juliet the sun. But you don't say

59:09 is the sun does that mean she's glowing ball of fire? I mean

59:13 do that But it's a different story ? Normal people say she's warm like

59:18 sun. She's radiant. Like the . She's nurturing like the sun instantly

59:21 the links. Now if you assume this greater cross wiring and concepts are

59:27 in different parts of the brain then going to create a greater propensity towards

59:32 thinking and creativity in people with synesthesia hence the eight times more common incidence

59:39 synesthesia among poets, artists and Ok. It's a very chronological view

59:43 synesthesia. The last demonstration can I one minute? Okay you're all sinister

59:52 but you're in denial about it. what I call martian alphabet. Just

59:56 your alphabet. A. Is B. S. B.

59:59 S. See different shapes for different . Right here you've got martian

60:04 One of them is kiki. One them is bouba. Which one is

60:08 ? And which one is about how of you think that's kiki? And

60:10 bouba. Mhm. How many of think the one on the ride is

60:17 and this one is bouba. How do you think that this is

60:25 Okay but raise your hands. Well one or two mutants? How many

60:33 you think that's bouba? That's kiki your hands 99% of you now none

60:37 you is a martian. How did do that? It's because you're all

60:41 a cross model synesthesia tick abstraction You're saying that that sharp inflection ki

60:48 your auditory cortex, the hair cells excited, kiki mimics the visual

60:54 sudden inflection of that jagged shape. this is very important because what it's

60:59 you is your brain is engaging in primitive, just it looks like a

61:03 illusion. But these photons in your are doing this shape and hair cells

61:08 your year are exciting the auditory pattern the brain is able to extract the

61:14 denominator. It's a primitive form of . And we now know this happens

61:19 the refusal form gyrus of the brain when that's damaged these people lose the

61:25 to engage in bouba kiki but they lose the ability to engage in

61:30 If you ask this guy what all glitters is not gold, what does

61:33 mean? The patient says well if metallic and shiny it doesn't mean it's

61:37 . You have to measure its specific . Okay, so they completely miss

61:41 metaphorical meaning. So this area is eight times the size in higher,

61:47 in humans as in lower primates. very interesting is going on here in

61:50 angular gyrus because it's the crossroads between , vision and touch enormous in humans

61:57 something very interesting is going on. I think it's the basis of many

62:01 human abilities like abstraction, metaphor and . All of these questions that philosophers

62:07 been studying for millennia. we scientists begin to explore by doing brain imaging

62:12 by studying patients and asking the right . Thank you. Like I

62:23 this is uh probably one of the interesting nurse ahn's ted talks that you

62:29 ever see and it will be on exam. All of these things that

62:34 written down here for those on You can see all of the things

62:38 we talked about, you know, grass syndrome, plant um limp synesthesia

62:45 areas of the brain are involved. is the explanation for the problem?

62:51 is the technique for the solution? atlantic skin response versus mirror box versus

62:57 of the trimming gene in synesthesia. thank you very much. I'm out

63:01 time today and good luck on the next week. You should be seeing

63:07 on casa, appearing in the next of days or so. Take care

63:10

-
+